Rohit,
Thanks for the timely pointer. The guy in the next cube has an
almost-two-year-old that is starting to read and is really into music.
Mike
> -----Original Message-----
> From: Rohit Khare [mailto:Rohit@KnowNow.com]
> Sent: Saturday, February 03, 2001 12:50 PM
> To: FoRK@xent.com
> Subject: "Rage to Master" from Dr. Winner
>
>
> >Just as Dr Ericsson took people with no discernible talent and
> >turned them into champions, so, in a fashion, did a Hungarian,
> >Laszlo Polgar. When he began training his daughters, it was widely
> >believed that women could not play serious tournament chess. But
> >through a deliberate (and still continuing) psychological
> >experiment, Dr Polgar and his wife created a trio of world-class
> >chess champions out of their own daughters, overturning this
> >prejudice.
> >
> >By 1992, all three had reached the women's top ten worldwide. The
> >third, who presumably received the most refined training regimen,
> >became the youngest grandmaster in the history of the game and is
> >reckoned by her peers to have a good chance of becoming world
> >champion one day. With remarkable, if not hubristic, prescience, Dr
> >Polgar had written a detailed book on the subject of child rearing,
> >entitled "Bring Up Genius!" before beginning the coaching of his
> >children. But would any child reared by such a parent have become a
> >chess prodigy?
> >
> >Ellen Winner, a psychologist at Boston College who has been studying
> >the relationship between exposure to the arts and subsequent
> >academic achievement, believes not. She argues that only children
> >with the "rage to master" a skill could make it through the
> >gruelling years of training needed to achieve expert ability. The
> >rage to master may be the point at which nature unequivocally makes
> >its constraints felt. Even Dr Ericsson concedes that there might be
> >a genetic component separating the child willing to persevere with a
> >rigorous schedule from the child who would rather play videogames.
> http://www.economist.com/displayStory.cfm?Story_ID=471563
>
>
> From the Devniad, Bob Devney:
> http://members.aol.com/~bobdevney/DEVAD27.html
> > BC psychologist Ellen Winner and her book Gifted Children:: Myths
> >and Realities (Basic Books, 1996).
> >
> >Here's a paragraph from the article that's sure to disappoint as
> >many as 99.9% of us, not-quite-Leonardos all.
> >
> >"Gifted children have three telltale characteristics, Winner says.
> >First, they begin to master an area of knowledge, or domain, such as
> >math, drawing or chess, at an extremely early age, before starting
> >school. Second, they need little help from adults in that domain,
> >solving problems in often-novel ways, with each discovery fueling
> >the next step. And third, they have what she describes as a rage to
> >master their domain, working at it intensively and obsessively,
> >often isolating themselves from others in order to pursue it. These
> >children push themselves, achieve "flow states" in their work, and
> >beg their parents for the books, musical instruments or art supplies
> >they need to feed their passion. They need stimulating environments
> >to develop their talents, Winner says of these children, but the
> >demand comes from them, not the parents."
> >
> >Her examples include Michael Kearney, who read signs and labels out
> >loud at the supermarket aged 10 months. (He's now the country's only
> >12-year-old graduate student in anthropology.) Or KyLee, who divined
> >the existence of prime numbers on his own at age 5.
> >
> >Sorry, friend. She's not talking here about when you begged Mom for
> >books on horses or Tom Swift when you were eight.
> >
> >Or, sad to say, even about me.
>
> http://www.edge.org/documents/archive/edge29.html
> >As Ellen Winner argues in Gifted Children (Basic Books, 1996),
> >prodigies can be distinguished from an early age from their peers.
> >Prodigies show a fascination (bordering on obsession) with a certain
> >content (e.g., numbers, visual patterns, auditory musical patterns)
> >and they have a rage to master the domains that deal with such
> >specific content. While they may have parental support, this support
> >is reactive rather than initiating. Moreover, prodigies - unlike the
> >rest of us - do not simply follow the conventional educational
> >pattern. They pose new questions, and they often solve domain issues
> >wholly or largely on their own. Philosopher Saul Kripke conjectures
> >that if algebra had not existed when he was in elementary school, he
> >would have invented it; and this kind of comment (whatever its truth
> >value in the specific case) captures quite accurately the mental
> >attitudes and powers of prodigies.
> >
> >No one understands the origins of prodigies. We simply have to
> >generate satisfying ways of thinking about them. I find it useful to
> >think of prodigies as having the same strategies and parameters with
> >reference to their chosen content domain that all normal individuals
> >have with respect to the mastery of one natural language. (In other
> >words, we are all linguistic prodigies, while prodigies in other
> >domains are rare). The prodigy seems "pretuned" to discover patterns
> >in the domain, including ones that have eluded others. Perhaps, if
> >it is to result in achievements that are valued by the adult
> >society, this gift has eventually to be wedded to strong motivation
> >(to succeed, to master) and to be creative (to step out in new
> >directions); and, if it is to be distinguished from the mechanistic
> >ability of the savant, it has eventually to be linked to wider
> >problems, including issues from other domains. Dean Keith Simonton
> >has written interestingly about the possibility that genius involves
> >the very occasional concatenation of these disparate human
> >proclivities and talents.
> >
> >I think that one is far more likely to understand Mozart, Bobby
> >Fischer, or Ramanujun if one assumes that they differ in fundamental
> >ways from the rest of the population than if one has to gerrymander
> >an explanation that simply builds on the general abilities of the
> >general public. Whether Ramanujun may have recalled an earlier feat
> >of calculation, and whether the rest of us could also recognize the
> >special features of the number 1729 is beside the point. Ramanujun
> >is honored because he covered several hundred years of mathematics
> >on his own in India and then made original contributions to number
> >theory after he joined G. H. Hardy in Cambridge.
>
> http://www.megafoundation.org/Ubiquity/May00/BookReview1_5.html
>
> Dr. Winner's Nine Myths About the Gifted.
> Myth #1: Giftedness, when it occurs, is generally global.
> The Reality: More often than not, children are unevenly gifted,often
> being especially gifted in one area. It's not uncommon to find them
> quite gifted in a specific area, but average or learning-disabled in
> another. (She gives the example of adult inventors with verbal IQ's
> of 60.)
>
> Myth 2: Talented children face different problems than
> gifted children.
> The Reality: Specially talented children face the same problems as
> the globally gifted.
>
> Myth 3: An exceptionally high IQ is required for giftedness.
> The Reality: Once the IQ exceeds 90, a high IQ is irrelevant in the
> fields of music and art.
>
> Myth 4: "Genius will out".
> The Reality: Families play a far more important role in the
> development of gifts than do schools, and are essential to the
> development of the gifted or talents child. Genius must be nurtured.
>
> Myth 5: Genius is entirely environmental.
> The Reality: The brains of the gifted are atypical. Their heads tend
> to be larger, their reflexes are faster, and their brains show
> atypical brain scan patterns. Brain structure, brain size, brain
> speed, brain efficiency, bilateral representation of language,
> language-related problems, non-right-handedness, immune system
> disorders. Programs such as the Japanese Suzuki Method of training
> students to play the violin can elicit remarkable results in
> children, but they don't produce musically gifted children. (Driven
> from within, prodigies are their own taskmasters. If anything, these
> programs testify to the biological basis of precocity.). Chinese
> drawing instruction produces the same kinds of dramatic juvenile
> output, but doesn't lead to true artistry, or to spontaneous learning
> of artistic principles.
>
> Myth 6: Prodigies are the result of parents that push their children.
> The Reality: Prodigies usually push their parents.
>
> Myth 7: Gifted children are glowing with psychological health.
> The Reality: As with a disability, giftedness can lead to unhappiness
> and social isolation. With adult minds in children's bodies,
> profoundly gifted children tend to be persecuted by other children.
> They tend to find little commonality with their age peers, relating
> to older children or adults.
>
> Myth 8: All children are gifted.
> The Reality: Nobody doubts that some children are musical or athletic
> prodigies. Nobody expects a small kid to become a tight end, or a
> short child to become a Harlem Globetrotter. Gifted children are
> biologically different. If you doubt it, try to raise someone's 90 IQ
> to 150. Dr. Winner cites the intriguing case of Charles, versus Eitan
> and Peter. All three boys were obsessed with drawing. However, Eitan
> and Peter were far ahead of their years, whereas Charles, in spite of
> all the drawing he did, never exceeded the norms for his age group.
>
> Myth 9: Gifted children become eminent adults.
> The Reality: Personality attributes more reliably predict what will
> happen in adulthood than does the child's degree of giftedness.
>
> Child prodigies are characterized by:
>
> * Precocity
> * Marching to their own drummers
> * A rage to master
>
> Dr. Winner cites two examples of global prodigies, "David" and
> Michael Kearney. David began to speak at eight months, and by fifteen
> months, knew 200 words. David learned to read at three, pushing his
> mother to show him what the words meant. Then he began to read
> voraciously, several books at a time. At five, he had reached a fifth
> grade reading level. At fifteen months, he could count to ten. At
> four, he could do simple two-digit additions in his head.
> One way to describe David and other super-bright children with a
> "rage to learn" is that they manipulate their environments in order
> to render them intellectually stimulating.
> Michael and Maeghan Kearney exemplify these characteristics.
> (Please see also the Book Review "Accidental Genius" in the Premier
> Issue of Ubiquity.) Michael began to talk at 4 months and to read at
> 10 months. He began high school at 5, and graduated from high school
> at 6, promptly entering San Joaquin Junior College. At 10, he
> graduated from the University of South Alabama with a 3.6 average in
> anthropology, and graduated from Middle Tennessee State University at
> 14 with a degree in chemistry. (He holds four academic records in the
> Guinness Book of World Records.) Maeghan is equally intelligent,
> although not as far advanced scholastically as Michael. Both Michael
> and Maeghan are globally gifted, and presumably, at the upper limit
> of the human register. Both of them exhibited a "rage to learn" and a
> need to stimulate their minds that was almost like a "magnificent
> addiction". Both of them pushed their parents. Their parents devoted
> all their resources to supporting their unique children, moving
> around the country and making the sacrifices necessary to nurture
> their children's gifts. The parents have gone all-out to ensure that
> Michael and Maeghan are as well-rounded and emotionally healthy as
> possible, and that they have had childhoods that are as normal as
> children this precocious can have.
>
> The Gifted Child:
>
> * Is very alert
> * Recognizes people at an early age
> * Has a preference for novelty
> * Is precocious in raising head, sitting up, walking, etc.
> * Talks early and well
> * Tends to be verreactive to noise, pain, frustration
> * Learns with minimal instruction
> * Is highly curious
> * Exhibits persistence and concentration
> * Possesses high energy
> * Has a metacognitive awareness. Induces rules if reading and
> math the way normal children induce the rules of syntax
> * Has obsessive interests
> * Tends to begin reading early and voraciously. Reading at
> 6th-grade level at 5 isn't unusual.
> * Is adept with numbers. Mathematically giftedness: numerical,
> spatial, and working memory tend to go tegether.
> * Has a good memory
> * Is proficient at abstract logical reasoning
> * Tends to have poor handwriting
> * Engages in solitary play (by default)
> * Prefers to associate with older children or adults
> * Exhibits philosophical and moral concerns
> * Possesses a good sense of humor
> * Experiences of awe
>
> The highly gifted child:
>
> * Occupies a special position within the family: often
> first-born or only children.
> * Grows up in "enriched" environments. Adam Konantovich.
> * Typically has child-centered parents. Yehudi Menuhin
> * Parents are driven.
> * Has parents who grant considerable independence.
> * Flourishes in an envrionment of high expectations and
> stimulation, combined with nurturance and support.
>
> When parents push too hard, the child may rebel or "burn out"
> Examples of this phenomenon are John Stuart Mill and William Sidis.
>
> Social and emotional problems
>
> * Is characterized by autonomy, independence of thought and
> values, will, and nonconformity.
> * Engages in advanced moral reasoning.
> * Tends toward introversion.
> * Has heightened sensitivity.
> * Loneliness
> * Lowered social self-Confidence
> * Does the label "Gifted" cause problems?
> * May underperform because they are underchallenged, and/or
> because they want social acceptance.
> * Enjoys a challenge
> * Sets high standards
> * Generally has academic self-esteem
>
> Gender differences:
> Boys with SAT math scores above 700 were 13 times as
> prevalent as
> girls. (However, the ratio is only 4:1 among Asian-Americans taking
> the SAT.).
>
> Terman Study
> The Terman-Cox Longitudinal (Lifetime) Study of Gifted Children
> began in 1921-22 with a screening of ~250,000 schoolchildren in
> California. Nominally, the top 1% were to be accepted into the study,
> but in reality, only 1,526, or (0.6%) were accepted. To compound the
> problem, the initial screening for the Terman Study was performed by
> teachers. We know today what they didn't know in 1921: that the
> brightest-seeming, best-behaved children may not be the brightest.
> The brightest may be bored troublemakers or argumentative with the
> teacher. In reality, the Terman Study selected much less than
> half--perhaps, 20%- of the children who would later become gifted
> adults. In particular, it missed the two children who would later
> become Nobel Laureates in physics--Dr. William Shockley and Dr. Luis
> Alvarez.
> Dr. Terman laid by the heels the adage, "Early to ripen,
> early to
> rot". For the most part, his "Termites" went on to become successful
> professionals. However, in his zeal to counter the pejorative notions
> about prodigies that pervaded the public mind, Dr. Terman went a
> little overboard. His data actually showed that the brighter the
> child, the less well-adjusted he/she. was. There was a "sweet spot"
> ranging from IQ 120 to, perhaps, IQ 150 where the individual is
> smarter than the average bear, but not so smart that they have
> problems adjusting to a lesser world--like the plight of a 6' 4" man
> versus that of his 7' counterpart.
>
> Gifted Programs
> Special Problems for Gifted Children
>
> * Because of their high energy levels and boredom with trivial
> busywork, gifted children are often misdiagnosed with ADHD.
> * Difficult to distinguish between boredom, disturbed, or
> learning disabled.
> * 30% show a discrepency between MA and reading achievement.
>
> Types of Gifted Programs
>
> * Egalitarianism.
> * Ability grouping
> * Acceleration
> * Home schooling is last resort. Can't be with their peers.
>
> Dr. Winner has this to say about our current offerings
> for gifted students:
>
> * American schools have low standards
> * Low standards lead to underachievement
> * School plays litle or no role in the nurturing of their gifts
> * Gifted chidren from disadvantaged backgrounds suffer most.
>
> Options:
>
> * Private schools
> * Magnet schools
> * Gifted programs
> * CTY
>
> In 1972, the Marland Report concluded that:
>
> * Only 4% of gifted students were getting any kind of
> special services
> * Half the superintendents said they had no gifted children in
> their schools.
> * Gifted are the most "retarded" in their schools because of
> discrepency between abilities and what schools could offer them.
> * Only disabled children have a law ma
Cambridge.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> <A =
HREF=3D"http://www.megafoundation.org/Ubiquity/May00/BookReview1_5.html"=
=
TARGET=3D"_blank">http://www.megafoundation.org/Ubiquity/May00/BookRevie=
w1_5.html</A></FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Dr. Winner's Nine Myths About the =
Gifted.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth #1: Giftedness, when it occurs, is =
generally global.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: More often than not, children are =
unevenly gifted,often </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> being especially gifted in one area. It's not =
uncommon to find them </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> quite gifted in a specific area, but average or =
learning-disabled in </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> another. (She gives the example of adult =
inventors with verbal IQ's </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> of 60.)</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 2: Talented children face different =
problems than </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> gifted children.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: Specially talented children face =
the same problems as </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> the globally gifted.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 3: An exceptionally high IQ is =
required for giftedness.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: Once the IQ exceeds 90, a high IQ =
is irrelevant in the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> fields of music and art.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 4: "Genius will out".</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: Families play a far more important =
role in the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> development of gifts than do schools, and are =
essential to the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> development of the gifted or talents child. =
Genius must be nurtured.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 5: Genius is entirely =
environmental.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: The brains of the gifted are =
atypical. Their heads tend </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> to be larger, their reflexes are faster, and =
their brains show </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> atypical brain scan patterns. Brain structure, =
brain size, brain </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> speed, brain efficiency, bilateral =
representation of language, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> language-related problems, =
non-right-handedness, immune system </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> disorders. Programs such as the Japanese Suzuki =
Method of training </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> students to play the violin can elicit =
remarkable results in </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> children, but they don't produce musically =
gifted children. (Driven </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> from within, prodigies are their own =
taskmasters. If anything, these </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> programs testify to the biological basis of =
precocity.). Chinese </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> drawing instruction produces the same kinds of =
dramatic juvenile </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> output, but doesn't lead to true artistry, or =
to spontaneous learning </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> of artistic principles.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 6: Prodigies are the result of =
parents that push their children.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: Prodigies usually push their =
parents.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 7: Gifted children are glowing with =
psychological health.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: As with a disability, giftedness =
can lead to unhappiness </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> and social isolation. With adult minds in =
children's bodies, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> profoundly gifted children tend to be =
persecuted by other children. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> They tend to find little commonality with their =
age peers, relating </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> to older children or adults.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 8: All children are gifted.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: Nobody doubts that some children =
are musical or athletic </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> prodigies. Nobody expects a small kid to become =
a tight end, or a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> short child to become a Harlem Globetrotter. =
Gifted children are </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> biologically different. If you doubt it, try to =
raise someone's 90 IQ </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> to 150. Dr. Winner cites the intriguing case of =
Charles, versus Eitan </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> and Peter. All three boys were obsessed with =
drawing. However, Eitan </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> and Peter were far ahead of their years, =
whereas Charles, in spite of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> all the drawing he did, never exceeded the =
norms for his age group.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Myth 9: Gifted children become eminent =
adults.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Reality: Personality attributes more =
reliably predict what will </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> happen in adulthood than does the child's =
degree of giftedness.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Child prodigies are characterized by:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Precocity</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Marching to their own =
drummers</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * A rage to =
master</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Dr. Winner cites =
two examples of global prodigies, "David" and </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Michael Kearney. David began to speak at eight =
months, and by fifteen </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> months, knew 200 words. David learned to read =
at three, pushing his </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> mother to show him what the words meant. Then =
he began to read </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> voraciously, several books at a time. At five, =
he had reached a fifth </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> grade reading level. At fifteen months, he =
could count to ten. At </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> four, he could do simple two-digit additions in =
his head.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> One way to =
describe David and other super-bright children with a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> "rage to learn" is that they =
manipulate their environments in order </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> to render them intellectually =
stimulating.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Michael and =
Maeghan Kearney exemplify these characteristics. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> (Please see also the Book Review =
"Accidental Genius" in the Premier </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Issue of Ubiquity.) Michael began to talk at 4 =
months and to read at </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> 10 months. He began high school at 5, and =
graduated from high school </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> at 6, promptly entering San Joaquin Junior =
College. At 10, he </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> graduated from the University of South Alabama =
with a 3.6 average in </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> anthropology, and graduated from Middle =
Tennessee State University at </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> 14 with a degree in chemistry. (He holds four =
academic records in the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Guinness Book of World Records.) Maeghan is =
equally intelligent, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> although not as far advanced scholastically as =
Michael. Both Michael </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> and Maeghan are globally gifted, and =
presumably, at the upper limit </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> of the human register. Both of them exhibited a =
"rage to learn" and a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> need to stimulate their minds that was almost =
like a "magnificent </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> addiction". Both of them pushed their =
parents. Their parents devoted </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> all their resources to supporting their unique =
children, moving </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> around the country and making the sacrifices =
necessary to nurture </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> their children's gifts. The parents have gone =
all-out to ensure that </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Michael and Maeghan are as well-rounded and =
emotionally healthy as </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> possible, and that they have had childhoods =
that are as normal as </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> children this precocious can have.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Gifted Child:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Is very alert</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Recognizes people at =
an early age</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Has a preference for =
novelty</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Is precocious in =
raising head, sitting up, walking, etc.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Talks early and =
well</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Tends to be =
verreactive to noise, pain, frustration</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Learns with minimal =
instruction</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Is highly =
curious</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Exhibits persistence =
and concentration</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Possesses high =
energy</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Has a metacognitive =
awareness. Induces rules if reading and </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> math the way normal children induce the rules =
of syntax</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Has obsessive =
interests</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Tends to begin =
reading early and voraciously. Reading at </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> 6th-grade level at 5 isn't unusual.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Is adept with =
numbers. Mathematically giftedness: numerical, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> spatial, and working memory tend to go =
tegether.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Has a good =
memory</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Is proficient at =
abstract logical reasoning</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Tends to have poor =
handwriting</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Engages in solitary =
play (by default)</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Prefers to associate =
with older children or adults</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Exhibits =
philosophical and moral concerns</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Possesses a good =
sense of humor</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Experiences of =
awe</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The highly gifted child:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Occupies a special =
position within the family: often </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> first-born or only children.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Grows up in =
"enriched" environments. Adam Konantovich.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Typically has =
child-centered parents. Yehudi Menuhin</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Parents are =
driven.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Has parents who grant =
considerable independence.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Flourishes in an =
envrionment of high expectations and </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> stimulation, combined with nurturance and =
support.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> When parents push too hard, the child may rebel =
or "burn out" </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Examples of this phenomenon are John Stuart =
Mill and William Sidis.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Social and emotional problems</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Is characterized by =
autonomy, independence of thought and </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> values, will, and nonconformity.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Engages in advanced =
moral reasoning.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Tends toward =
introversion.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Has heightened =
sensitivity.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Loneliness</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Lowered social =
self-Confidence</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Does the label =
"Gifted" cause problems?</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * May underperform =
because they are underchallenged, and/or </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> because they want social acceptance.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Enjoys a =
challenge</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Sets high =
standards</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Generally has =
academic self-esteem</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Gender differences:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Boys with SAT =
math scores above 700 were 13 times as </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> prevalent as </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> girls. (However, the ratio is only 4:1 among =
Asian-Americans taking </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> the SAT.).</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Terman Study</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Terman-Cox =
Longitudinal (Lifetime) Study of Gifted Children </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> began in 1921-22 with a screening of ~250,000 =
schoolchildren in </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> California. Nominally, the top 1% were to be =
accepted into the study, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> but in reality, only 1,526, or (0.6%) were =
accepted. To compound the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> problem, the initial screening for the Terman =
Study was performed by </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> teachers. We know today what they didn't know =
in 1921: that the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> brightest-seeming, best-behaved children may =
not be the brightest. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The brightest may be bored troublemakers or =
argumentative with the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> teacher. In reality, the Terman Study selected =
much less than </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> half--perhaps, 20%- of the children who would =
later become gifted </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> adults. In particular, it missed the two =
children who would later </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> become Nobel Laureates in physics--Dr. William =
Shockley and Dr. Luis </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Alvarez.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Dr. Terman laid =
by the heels the adage, "Early to ripen, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> early to </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> rot". For the most part, his =
"Termites" went on to become successful </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> professionals. However, in his zeal to counter =
the pejorative notions </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> about prodigies that pervaded the public mind, =
Dr. Terman went a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> little overboard. His data actually showed that =
the brighter the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> child, the less well-adjusted he/she. was. =
There was a "sweet spot" </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> ranging from IQ 120 to, perhaps, IQ 150 where =
the individual is </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> smarter than the average bear, but not so smart =
that they have </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> problems adjusting to a lesser world--like the =
plight of a 6' 4" man </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> versus that of his 7' counterpart.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Gifted Programs</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Special Problems for Gifted Children</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Because of their high =
energy levels and boredom with trivial </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> busywork, gifted children are often =
misdiagnosed with ADHD.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Difficult to =
distinguish between boredom, disturbed, or </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> learning disabled.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * 30% show a =
discrepency between MA and reading achievement.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Types of Gifted Programs</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * =
Egalitarianism.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Ability =
grouping</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Acceleration</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Home schooling is =
last resort. Can't be with their peers.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Dr. Winner has =
this to say about our current offerings </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> for gifted students:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * American schools have =
low standards</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Low standards lead to =
underachievement</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * School plays litle or =
no role in the nurturing of their gifts</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Gifted chidren from =
disadvantaged backgrounds suffer most.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Options:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Private =
schools</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Magnet schools</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Gifted =
programs</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * CTY</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> In 1972, the Marland Report concluded =
that:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Only 4% of gifted =
students were getting any kind of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> special services</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Half the =
superintendents said they had no gifted children in </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> their schools.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Gifted are the most =
"retarded" in their schools because of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> discrepency between abilities and what schools =
could offer them.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Only disabled =
children have a law mandating that they get </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> special educational treatment. Only about a =
fifth of our states </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> include the gifted as special education =
students covered by the law </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> for the handicapped.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Riley Report, a Follow-Up Study to the =
Marland Report:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Again deplored the =
state of gifted education in our country.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Observed that we =
offer far more services to retarded children </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> than to gifted children.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * IQ's 2 s. d. below =
the mean (68) are given special help.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * IQ's 3 s. d. below =
the mean (52) are enrolled in partial or </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> full-day programs</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * IQ's 4 s. d. below =
the mean (36) are given special </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> supervision and are in institutions.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Dr. Winner Concludes that:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * We should pull up all =
school standards.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * We are wasting what =
few gifted resources we have.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> The Gifted Child Grows Up</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Benjamin Bloom: Not one world-class performer =
in a variety of fields, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> including math, art, music, and athletics ever =
achieved expertise </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> without a supportive and encouraging =
environment, including a long </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> and intensive period of training, first from =
loving and warm </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> teachers, and then from demanding and rigorous =
master teachers.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Anders Ericsson: Levels of achievement reached =
in piano, violin, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> chess, bridge, and athletics correlate highly =
with hours of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> deliberate practice.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> (Shows necessity but not sufficiency.)</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Childhood: 30% hereditary, 30% family =
environment, 40% other </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> environmental.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Adolesence: 50% hereditary, 10% familial, with =
40% environmental.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Adulthood: 75% heredity, 5% childhood =
background, 15% environmental, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> 15% error.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Eitan lost his passion for art.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Out of 70 musical prodigies in San Francisco in =
the 20's and 30's, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> only 6 (including Yehudi Menuhin and Leon =
Fischer) went on to become </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> well-known soloists. Norbert Wiener, Jean =
Piaget, and Pablo Picasso </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> went on to become highly successful adults. =
There are four classes of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> outcomes:</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Gifted children who =
drop out. Wiener made it; Sidis didn't.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Gifted children who =
become experts, but not creative geniuses.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Gifted children who =
become adult geniuses. Not only early </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> ability but a rebellious disposition.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> * Late bloomers. Bill =
Gates, Edwin Land, Buckminster Fuller</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Terman subjects were too well-adjusted.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Adult geniuses stand out far more clearly in =
personality and </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> motivational factors.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Above an IQ of 120, there is no relationshiip =
between IQ and genius. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Some inventors have verbal IQ's as low as 60. =
Shockley and Alvarez. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Marilyn vos Savant. IQ tests tell nothing about =
social skills, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> intrapersonal skills, "practical" =
intelligence, and resilience. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> (Quotation about high-IQ societies.)</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Greatest classical composers tended to have =
been child prodigies. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Prodigies take about three fewer years to =
achieve greatness, and they </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> tend to achieve g
Ericsson also maintains </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> that such memory function, and the superlative =
performance that goes </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> with it, can be attained by anyone-biology no =
bar-given enough </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> practice and perseverance.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> This is a much more contentious point. Twenty =
years ago, Dr Ericsson </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> tried to prove it by training some ordinary =
laboratory volunteers up </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> to prodigy-level performance in a number-memory =
task. Average people </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> tend to have a "digit-span" of =
seven-in other words they can recall a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> string of seven random digits after hearing it =
read out once. But </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> after a year's practice, two of his =
particularly dedicated subjects </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> were able to increase their digit-spans to =
lengths of 80 and 100.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Just as Dr Ericsson took people with no =
discernible talent and turned </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> them into champions, so, in a fashion, did a =
Hungarian, Laszlo </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Polgar. When he began training his daughters, =
it was widely believed </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> that women could not play serious tournament =
chess. But through a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> deliberate (and still continuing) psychological =
experiment, Dr Polgar </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> and his wife created a trio of world-class =
chess champions out of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> their own daughters, overturning this =
prejudice.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> By 1992, all three had reached the women's top =
ten worldwide. The </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> third, who presumably received the most refined =
training regimen, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> became the youngest grandmaster in the history =
of the game and is </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> reckoned by her peers to have a good chance of =
becoming world </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> champion one day. With remarkable, if not =
hubristic, prescience, Dr </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Polgar had written a detailed book on the =
subject of child rearing, </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> entitled "Bring Up Genius!" before =
beginning the coaching of his </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> children. But would any child reared by such a =
parent have become a </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> chess prodigy?</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Ellen Winner, a psychologist at Boston College =
who has been studying </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> the relationship between exposure to the arts =
and subsequent academic </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> achievement, believes not. She argues that only =
children with the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> "rage to master" a skill could make =
it through the gruelling years of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> training needed to achieve expert ability. The =
rage to master may be </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> the point at which nature unequivocally makes =
its constraints felt. </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Even Dr Ericsson concedes that there might be a =
genetic component </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> separating the child willing to persevere with =
a rigorous schedule </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> from the child who would rather play =
videogames.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Put it another way: even if there are no born =
mathematicians or </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> musicians, there may be "born =
achievers". The particular area in </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> which such people make their mark might be =
determined purely by the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> kind of environment or skill to which they were =
exposed and how hard </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> they then applied themselves. But among many =
psychologists this </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> all-purpose view of genius is not a popular =
one. Dean Simonton of the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> University of California, San Diego, dubbed it =
the "drudge theory" of </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> genius in a recent book review.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Dr Simonton considers genius to have more of a =
genetic component. Yet </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> this conviction has not stopped him from =
writing a book of profiles </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> of psychologists who were reckoned to be =
geniuses. The American </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Psychological Association will publish this =
book later this year, so </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> that its members may learn from Dr Simonton's =
observations on the </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> great prodigies of psychology. And though Dr =
Ericsson is not on his </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> list this year, in ten years from now he =
doubtless will be-if he </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> wants it badly enough.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> Copyright =A9 1995-2001 The Economist Newspaper =
Group Ltd. All </FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> rights reserved.</FONT>
<BR><FONT SIZE=3D2>> </FONT>
</P>
</BODY>
</HTML>
This archive was generated by hypermail 2b29 : Fri Apr 27 2001 - 23:17:27 PDT